Cognitive and Emotional Effects of Bilingualism Childhood and Adulthood

Umi Farisiyah (1)
(1) Madrasah Aliyah Al Hikmah 2, Brebes, Central Java, Indonesia

Abstract

Mastering more than one languages gives some advantages because of the correlation of the language and the brain. If we use more than one languages mean our brain has to work more that it works in one language. It means our brain has to be more active that can activate all the neurons of the brain and automatically make the brain much better in its functions. And of course, using more than one languages change how we assume and interpret everything in our surrounding. Using evidences from reaction time, eye-tracking, functional magnetic resonance imaging, and event-related potential research, it shows how language experience transforms cognitive control and emotion, two features that they are central to human communication. We suggest that managing multiple languages not only impacts cognitive control and emotion independently, but also the way in which they interact with each other. Within cognitive control, we present research suggesting that bilinguals demonstrate benefits compared to monolinguals and discuss the potential sources of these benefits, including parallel language activation and language switching. We also explore the potential links between linguistic and non-linguistic cognitive processes. Within emotion, we discuss whether bilinguals process emotions similarly across their first and second languages and consider how linguistic context and one’s cultural affiliation may impact memory (e.g., storage and retrieval) of emotional events. The evidence discussed in this chapter highlights the transformative effect that bilingual experience has on how human beings understand and interpret the world

Full text article

Generated from XML file

References

Benjafield. (1992). Cognition. New Jersey: Prentice Hall Inc.

Berns, Margie. (2010). Concise Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics. London: Elsevier Ltd.

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(4), 859–873.

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303.

Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9(1), 103–119.

Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2007). Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(5), 633–660.

________. (2013). Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 547– 567.

Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in cognitive sciences, 4(6), 215-222.

Ferré, P., García, T., Fraga, I., Sánchez-Casas, R., & Molero, M. (2010). Memory for emotional words in bilinguals: Do words have the same emotional intensity in the first and in the second language? Cognition and Emotion, 24(5), 760–785.

Freeman, Max R., Shook, A., and Marian, V. (2016) Cognitive and emotional effects of bilingualism in adulthood. Lifespan Perspectives on Bilingualism.

Hernandez, A. E. (2009). Language switching in the bilingual brain: What’s next? Brain and Language, 109(2), 133-140.

Kroll, J. F. (2009). The consequences of bilingualism for the mind and the brain. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10(3), i–ii.

Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 497–514.

Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming. Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 149-174.

Ling, Jonathan & Catling, J. (2012). Cognitive Psychology. London: Pearson Education Limited.

Marian, V., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2004). Self-construal and emotion in bicultural bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(2), 190–201.

_______. (2007). Language context guides memory content. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(5), 925-933.

Marian, V., & Neisser, U. (2000). Language-dependent recall of autobiographical memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 129(3), 361-368.

Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(01), 81– 93.

Pavlenko, A. (2008). Emotion and emotion-laden words in the bilingual lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(2), 147–164.

________. (2012). Affective processing in bilingual speakers: Disembodied cognition? International Journal of Psychology, 47(6), 405–428.

Schunk, D.H. (2012). Learning Theories. London: Pearson Education Limited.

Sutton, T. M., Altarriba, J., Gianico, J. L., & Basnight-Brown, D. M. (2007). The automatic access of emotion: Emotional Stroop effects in Spanish–English bilingual speakers. Cognition and Emotion, 21(5), 1077–1090.

Authors

Umi Farisiyah
farisiyahumi@yahoo.com (Primary Contact)
Author Biography

Umi Farisiyah, Madrasah Aliyah Al Hikmah 2, Brebes, Central Java

English teacher
Farisiyah, U. (2018). Cognitive and Emotional Effects of Bilingualism Childhood and Adulthood. Tell : Teaching of English Language and Literature Journal, 6(1), 21–29. https://doi.org/10.30651/tell.v6i1.2076

Article Details